An integrated theory of whole number and fractions development RS Siegler, CA Thompson, M Schneider Cognitive psychology 62 (4), 273-296, 2011 | 1015 | 2011 |
Relations of different types of numerical magnitude representations to each other and to mathematics achievement LK Fazio, DH Bailey, CA Thompson, RS Siegler Journal of experimental child psychology 123, 53-72, 2014 | 661 | 2014 |
How 15 hundred is like 15 cherries: Effect of progressive alignment on representational changes in numerical cognition CA Thompson, JE Opfer Child Development 81 (6), 1768-1786, 2010 | 275 | 2010 |
The logarithmic‐to‐linear shift: One learning sequence, many tasks, many time scales RS Siegler, CA Thompson, JE Opfer Mind, Brain, and Education 3 (3), 143-150, 2009 | 230 | 2009 |
Early development of spatial‐numeric associations: evidence from spatial and quantitative performance of preschoolers JE Opfer, CA Thompson, EE Furlong Developmental Science 13 (5), 761-771, 2010 | 205 | 2010 |
Linear numerical-magnitude representations aid children’s memory for numbers CA Thompson, RS Siegler Psychological Science 21 (9), 1274-1281, 2010 | 148 | 2010 |
Costs and benefits of representational change: Effects of context on age and sex differences in symbolic magnitude estimation CA Thompson, JE Opfer Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 101 (1), 20-51, 2008 | 147 | 2008 |
Children are not like older adults: A diffusion model analysis of developmental changes in speeded responses R Ratcliff, J Love, CA Thompson, JE Opfer Child development 83 (1), 367-381, 2012 | 140 | 2012 |
The trouble with transfer: Insights from microgenetic changes in the representation of numerical magnitude JE Opfer, CA Thompson Child Development 79 (3), 788-804, 2008 | 122 | 2008 |
Modeling individual differences in response time and accuracy in numeracy R Ratcliff, CA Thompson, G McKoon Cognition 137, 115-136, 2015 | 97 | 2015 |
Numerical landmarks are useful—Except when they’re not RS Siegler, CA Thompson Journal of experimental child psychology 120, 39-58, 2014 | 80 | 2014 |
Free versus anchored numerical estimation: A unified approach JE Opfer, CA Thompson, D Kim Cognition 149, 11-17, 2016 | 74 | 2016 |
Student perceptions of general education requirements at a large public university: No surprises? CA Thompson, M Eodice, P Tran The Journal of General Education 64 (4), 278-293, 2015 | 70 | 2015 |
Number lines, but not area models, support children’s accuracy and conceptual models of fraction division PG Sidney, CA Thompson, FD Rivera Contemporary Educational Psychology 58, 288-298, 2019 | 69 | 2019 |
Who uses more strategies? Linking mathematics anxiety to adults’ strategy variability and performance on fraction magnitude tasks PG Sidney, R Thalluri, ML Buerke, CA Thompson Thinking & Reasoning 25 (1), 94-131, 2019 | 57 | 2019 |
Children’s and adults’ math attitudes are differentiated by number type PG Sidney, CA Thompson, C Fitzsimmons, JM Taber The Journal of Experimental Education 89 (1), 1-32, 2021 | 56 | 2021 |
Even early representations of numerical magnitude are spatially organized: Evidence for a directional magnitude bias in pre-reading preschoolers JE Opfer, CA Thompson Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society 28 (28), 2006 | 53 | 2006 |
The logarithmic-to-linear shift: One learning sequence, many tasks, many time scales. Mind, Brain, and Education, 3 (3), 143–150 RS Siegler, CA Thompson, JE Opfer | 52 | 2009 |
Do adults treat equivalent fractions equally? Adults’ strategies and errors during fraction reasoning. CJ Fitzsimmons, CA Thompson, PG Sidney Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 46 (11 …, 2020 | 49 | 2020 |
Associations of magnitude comparison and number line estimation with mathematical competence: A comparative review M Schneider, CA Thompson, B Rittle-Johnson Cognitive development from a strategy perspective, 100-119, 2017 | 42 | 2017 |